Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive Load Theory as a Tool for Expertise Development

  • Commentary
  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This special issue is dedicated to recentdevelopments within cognitive load theory (CLT)and identifies some instructional implicationsof the interaction between informationstructures and cognitive architecture. Thepresent article discusses the different studiesin this special issue. An important conclusionis that recent CLT research recognizes thelearners' level of expertise as an importantfactor mediating the relation between cognitivearchitecture, information structures, andlearning outcomes. In this context, we discussthe role of expertise in CLT research and therole of CLT in expertise research. Based onthis discussion, the use of CLT as a tool todevelop expertise is identified as a verypromising approach.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

REFERENCES

  • Atkinson, R.K., Derry, S.D., Renkl, A. & Wortham, D.W. (2000) Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research, Review of Educational Research 70: 181–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S.L., Carr, T.H., MacMahon, C. & Starkes, J.L. (2002) When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 8: 6–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S.L., Wierenga, S.A. & Carr, T.H. (2002) Expertise, attention, and memory in sensorimotor skill execution: Impact of novel task constraints on dual-task performance and episodic memory, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 55: 1211–1240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camp, G., Paas, F., Rikers, R. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2001) Dynamic problem selection in air traffic control training: A comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency, Computers in Human Behavior 17: 575–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P. & Glaser, R. (1989) Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems, Cognitive Science 13: 145–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A., Chase, W.G. & Faloon, S. (1980) Acquisition of a memory skill, Science 208: 1181–1182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.Th. & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993) The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance, Psychological Review 3: 363–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A. & Lehmann, A.C. (1996) Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints, Annual Review of Psychology 47: 273–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K. & Catrambone, R. (2004) Designing instructional examples to reduce intrinsic cognitive load: Molar versus modular presentation of solution procedures, Instructional Science 32(1–2): 33–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinds, P., Patterson, M. & Pfeffer, J. (2001) Bothered by abstraction: The effect of expertise on knowledge transfer and subsequent novice performance, Journal of Applied Psychology 86: 1232–1243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (2003) The expertise reversal effect, Educational Psychologist 38: 23–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (1998) Levels of expertise and instructional design, Human Factors 40: 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R. & Mayer, R.E. (2000) A coherence effect in multimedia learning: The case for minimizing irrelevant sounds in the design of multimedia instructional messages, Journal of Educational Psychology 92: 117–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. (1992) Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach, Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 429–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1994) Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive load approach, Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 122–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A. & Sweller, J. (2003a) Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments, Educational Psychologist 38: 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A. & Sweller, J. (2003b) Cognitive load theory (special issue), Educational Psychologist 38.

  • Patel, V.L. & Groen, G.J. (1986) Knowledge based solution strategies in medical reasoning, Cognitive Science 10: 91–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, V.L. & Groen, G.J. (1991) The general and specific nature of medical expertise: A critical look. In: A. Ericsson and J. Smith (eds), Toward a General Theory of Expertise: Prospects and Limits, pp. 93–125. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1997) Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences, Cognitive Science 21: 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. & Atkinson, R.K. (2003) Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skills acquisition: A cognitive load perspective, Educational Psychologist 38: 15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Atkinson, R.K. & Große, C.S. (2004) How fading worked solution steps works–A cognitive load perspective, Instructional Science 32(1–2): 59–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Atkinson, R.K., Maier, U.H. & Staley, R. (2002) From example study to problem solving: Smooth transitions help learning, Journal of Experimental Education 70: 293–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salden, R.J.C.M., Paas, F., Broers, N.J. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004) Mental effort and performance as determinants for the dynamic selection of learning tasks in air traffic control training, Instructional Science 32(1–2): 153–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988) Cognitive load during problem solving, Cognitive Science 12: 257–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2004) Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture, Instructional Science 32(1–2): 9–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Paas, F. (1998) Cognitive architecture and instructional design, Educational Psychology Review 10: 251–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuovinen, J.E. & Paas, F. (2004) Exploring multidimensional approaches to the efficiency of instructional conditions, Instructional Science 32(1–2): 133–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Paas, F. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004) Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding, Instructional Science 32(1–2): 83–98.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Remy M.J.P. Rikers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rikers, R.M., Van Gerven, P.W. & Schmidt, H.G. Cognitive Load Theory as a Tool for Expertise Development. Instructional Science 32, 173–182 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021807.49315.31

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021807.49315.31

Navigation