2012-10-01
Binocular eye movement control and motion perception: What is being tracked?
Publication
Publication
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science , Volume 53 - Issue 11 p. 7268- 7275
PURPOSE. We investigated under what conditions humans can make independent slow phase eye movements. The ability to make independent movements of the two eyes generally is attributed to few specialized lateral eyed animal species, for example chameleons. In our study, we showed that humans also can move the eyes in different directions. To maintain binocular retinal correspondence independent slow phase movements of each eye are produced. METHODS. We used the scleral search coil method to measure binocular eye movements in response to dichoptically viewed visual stimuli oscillating in orthogonal direction. RESULTS. Correlated stimuli led to orthogonal slow eye movements, while the binocularly perceived motion was the vector sum of the motion presented to each eye. The importance of binocular fusion on independency of the movements of the two eyes was investigated with anticorrelated stimuli. The perceived global motion pattern of anti-correlated dichoptic stimuli was perceived as an oblique oscillatory motion, as well as resulted in a conjugate oblique motion of the eyes. CONCLUSIONS. We propose that the ability to make independent slow phase eye movements in humans is used to maintain binocular retinal correspondence. Eye-of-origin and binocular information are used during the processing of binocular visual information, and it is decided at an early stage whether binocular or monocular motion information and independent slow phase eye movements of each eye are produced during binocular tracking.
Additional Metadata | |
---|---|
doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-9809, hdl.handle.net/1765/61875 | |
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science | |
Organisation | Department of Neuroscience |
van der Steen, H., & Dits, J. (2012). Binocular eye movement control and motion perception: What is being tracked?. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 53(11), 7268–7275. doi:10.1167/iovs.12-9809 |